close
test_template

Reduction of Inhibitory Control in People with ADHD

Human-Written
download print

About this sample

About this sample

close
Human-Written

Words: 2293 |

Pages: 5|

12 min read

Published: Mar 18, 2021

Essay grade:
Good
arrow downward Read Review

Words: 2293|Pages: 5|12 min read

Published: Mar 18, 2021

Essay grade:
Good
arrow downward Read Review

Inhibitory control is the ability to voluntarily inhibit prepotent attentional or behavioural responses. Per Barkley (1997a), inhibitory control is the basis of proper functioning of all the executive functions, as well as it being one of the most used cognitive function. It is the way in which the brain corrects a behaviour. It makes it possible to stay quiet when you want to say something inappropriate, to avoid negative thoughts and focus on the positive, to not scratch that bite even though it is itching. Inhibitory abilities have been examined in the laboratory in great depth, using experiments such as ‘The Marshmallow Experiment’ – where children must use inhibitory control to resist eating one marshmallow in order to get two marshmallows – or the Stroop task – a task in which you must say the colour of the word rather than the actual word. In both tasks, you must inhibit the normal response of eating the marshmallow or reading the word. It has been suggested by many psychologists that is a deficit in inhibitory control that causes the behavioural symptoms associated with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.

This essay aims to define and describe Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) before recognising existing theories accounting for inhibitory control deficits in ADHD and analysing current research that provides evidence for these deficits. ADHD is a behavioural disorder in which the core symptoms are defined as “inattentiveness, impulsivity and hyperactivity”. Current research throughout westernised cultures shows that between 3-5% of school aged children display hyperactive, impulsive and inattentive behaviours severe enough to meet the guidelines issued in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV to be diagnosed with Attention Deficit Disorder (ADHD). In addition to this, many children experience similar kinds of behaviours without meeting the full criteria for an ADHD diagnosis, making these the most common of all behavioural problems in children (Barkley, 1998). The DSM-IV identifies three different subtypes of ADHD: the combined type, the predominantly hyperactive type and the predominantly inattentive type. In the combined type, a child must display at least six of nine symptoms of hyperactivity, as well as six of nine symptoms of inattention. For the other two subtypes, the child needs only to meet the criteria for one or the other.

There have been theories presented to account for the various inhibitory deficits associated with ADHD. The most comprehensive of these models is the hybrid model of ADHD proposed by Barkley (1997a). According to this model, ADHD is seen mainly as a deficit in inhibitory control. In addition to this, Barkley’s perspective is that inhibition is primary to other executive functions – stating that the first action must always be to inhibit one response, causing a delay in which other executive functions can occur. Stemming from this view, Barkley nominates the ideal what children with ADHD have further problems with different executive functions, these being: non-verbal working memory (the ability to hold an event in mind and use it to control a response), internalisation of speech (more commonly known as verbal working memory, it is short term memory based on what we hear), self-regulation of affect, arousal and motivation (an action an individual directs themselves to result in a change of behaviour or change a future outcome) and reconstitution (the analysis and synthesis of sequences of events into parts, and manipulation these parts, to then reconstruct new events). Therefore, he organised his model in a hierarchical way with a reduced inhibitory control at the top, leading to a reduction in other executive functions below it, all leading to an account of the deficits associated with ADHD, as presented in the model as Motor Control/fluency/syntax. Despite Barkley’s model being the most influential, there are still competing theories that must be acknowledged.

In 1982 Gray presented the Theory of BIS and BAS. This is a neuropsychological theory consisting of three systems that interact with one another: the behavioural activation system (BAS), the behavioural inhibition system (BIS) and the nonspecific arousal system (NAS). The BAS responds to conditioned stimuli for a reward and/or a relief of punishment, the BIS responds to conditioned stimuli for novelty, as well as punishment and a no reward. The NAS responds to unconditional pain and punishment, and is a fight or flight system. Gray (1982) initially used this as a basis for explaining anxiety problems which he stated was as a result of an overactive BIS. In 1997, Quay used this theory to explain the deficits used in ADHD, suggesting that they are associated with an underactive BIS. The fact that anxiety problems and ADHD are both related to the BIS, in opposite directions suggests that the two disorders cannot co-occur, yet around a quarter of those diagnosed with ADHD also meet the criteria for anxiety disorders (Cohen et al. 1993). When explaining, this finding, the BIS, as described by Gray is an inhibitory system that is linked to punishment and reward, completely different to inhibition as described by Barkley. In a working taxonomy of inhibition presented by Nigg (2000), BIS is referred to as a type of motivational inhibition whereas Barkley’s definition of inhibition is referred to as executive inhibition. Making this distinction is important, as it implies that a measure of BIS functioning can only be when executive tasks are performed under motivational conditionsand when the distinction in the types of inhibition is made the evidence of an underactive BIS in ADHD is limited.

Another theory as to how a deficit in inhibition links to ADHD is in Rothbarts theory of effortful control. This evaluates the roll of two different systems, the fear/behavioural inhibition system and effortful control. The fear/behavioural system is a motivational system, but also has modulatory effects via connections with other systems. It is thought to suppress reward-orientated behaviours. Effortful control is an active control system, which is self-regulatory. It allows a dominant response to be inhibited for a subdominant response to be performed. In ADHD, a weakened fear regulation could potentially result in impulsive behaviour, especially as the individual is unable to constrain their behaviour through the system of effortful control.

In terms of research that supports the theories, studies showing a reduced inhibition in those with ADHD are plentiful. For the most part, research is based on Barkley’s model of deficits within ADHD showing lower levels of executive inhibition, as well as a deficit of the other four executive functions. When looking at executive inhibition it has been those with ADHD have lower levels of impulse control as compared to control groups when tested using either stop-signal tasks or the go/no-go paradigm. It has also been shown in two separate studies carried out by Dunn et al. (1998) that poor inhibition is related to hyperactivity. When examining interference control - a type of control that helps filter out distracting information or supress habits or responses that were irrelevant. For tasks with external distractions, those with ADHD are unlikely to differentiate in any way from normal controls. However, when the distractions are inserted into the task, such as a strop task those with ADHD perform significantly worse than those who don’t.

When looking specifically at the other types of executive function included in Barkley’s model, there is many deficits observed in children with ADHD. Through tasks of memory for special location it has been shown that there are significant differences in the non-verbal working memory in children with AHDH. In regards to verbal working memory Mariani and Barkley (1997) used repetition of digit spans to demonstrate a deficit among ADHD sufferers. When a child has severe ADHD symptoms it has been shown that they have delayed internalisation of speech. Further support of deficits in the executive functions in Barkley’s model come from studies that show significant difference between ADHD and controls regarding measures of self-regulation of affect and verbal fluency. It is however important to discuss the studies that have failed to find any significant group differences such as working memory and verbal fluency. On top of this, when evaluating Barkley’s model, it is crucial to consider the studies that have failed to find any significant group differences for inhibition.

Building upon the theoretical and experimental support for inhibitory control there is also several biological studies suggesting there are differences in the physiology of the brain between ADHD and norm. The localizationist theory has associated inhibition with various areas of the frontal cortex. It has also been indicated that Inhibitory control begins emerging within the first postnatal year, continuing to develop through toddler and into preschool years; this pattern is simultaneous with changes in the maturation of the frontal lobe. Structural and functional neuroimaging studies on subjects with ADHD show various abnormalities associated with the disorder. In Structural imaging studies, it has been shown that maturation of the brain is delayed during ADHD, and functional imaging studies indicate abnormalities are predominantly in the frontal cortex (Sowell et al 2003).

Get a custom paper now from our expert writers.

Using points described through my discussion, I can conclude that theoretical, experimental and neurobiological research generally support the statement that inhibitory control is significantly reduced in those with ADHD. It is however in debate as to how the deficit in inhibitory control occurs in people diagnosed with ADHD making this an area of psychology that would benefit from further research.

References

  1. AD/HD+CD, anxious, and control children: A meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 39, 411-425.
  2. ADHD: Clinical Practice Guideline for the Diagnosis, Evaluation, and Treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adolescents. (2011). PEDIATRICS, 128(5), 1007-1022. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2654
  3. Aron, A., Robbins, T., & Poldrack, R. (2014). Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends In Cognitive Sciences, 18(4), 177-185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003
  4. Barkley, R. A. (1997a). ADHD and the nature of self -control. New York: Guilford Press. Barkley, R. A. (1997b). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65-94.
  5. Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (2nd ed). New York Guildford Press.
  6. Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G., & DuPaul, G. (1992). Frontal lobe functions in attention deficit disor- der with and without hyperactivity: A review and a research report. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20, 163-188.
  7. Berk, L. E. & Landau, S (1993). Private speech of learning disabled and normally achieving children in classroom academic and laboratory contexts. Child Development, 64, 556-571.
  8. Cohen, P., Cohen, J., Kasen, S. et al. (1993). An epidemiological study of disorders in late childhood and adolescence, I: age and gender specific pattern. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 851-867.
  9. Davenport, J. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (IV). Washington, D.C: America Psychiatric Ass.
  10. Derryberry, D. & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of tem- perament, Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633-652.
  11. Diamond, Adele. (2002). Normal Development of Prefrontal Cortex from Birth to Young Adulthood: Cognitive Functions, Anatomy, and Biochemistry. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0029.
  12. Douglas, V. I. (1983). Attention and cognitive problems. In M. Rutter (Ed.), Developmental Neuro- psychiatry (pp. 280-329). New York: Guilford Press.
  13. Douglas, V. I. (1988). Cognitive deficits in children with Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperacti- vity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry (Monograph Suppl.), 65-81.
  14. Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Edelbrock, C., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: II. Academic, attentional, and neuropsychological status. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 580-588.
  15. Gray, J. A. (1982). The neuropsychology of anxiety . New York: Oxford University Press.
  16. Hughes, C., Dunn, J., & White, A. (1998). Trick or treat?: Uneven understanding of mind and emotion and executive function in “hard-to-manage” preschoolers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 981-994.
  17. Hughes, C., White, A., Sharpen, J., & Dunn, J. (2000). Antisocial, angry, and unsympathetic: “Hard- to-manage” preschoolers peer problems and possible cognitive influences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 169-179.
  18. Kerns, K. A., McInterney, R. J., & Wilde, N. J. (2001). Time reproduction, working memory, and behavioral inhibition in children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychology, 7, 21-31
  19. Mariani, M. & Barkley, R. A. (1997). Neuropsychological and academic functioning in preschool children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 111- 129.
  20. Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal Of Personality And Social Psychology, 16(2), 329-337. doi: 10.1037/h0029815
  21. Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 220-246.
  22. Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127, 571-598.
  23. Oosterlaan, J., Logan, G. D., & Sergeant, J. A. (1998). Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid
  24. Quay, H. C. (1997). Inhibition and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25, 7-13.
  25. R. Barkay (2019). Retrieved from http://www.russellbarkley.org/factsheets/ADHD_EF_and_SR.pdf
  26. Rothbart, M. K. & Bates, J. E. (1998). Temperament. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds.) Handbook of child psychology (Fifth Edition). Volume 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp. 105-176). New York, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
  27. Schachar, R., & Logan, G. (1990). Impulsivity and inhibitory control in normal development and childhood psychopathology, Developmental Psychology, 26, 710-720.
  28. Shue, K. L. & Douglas, V. I. (1992). Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and the frontal lobe syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 20, 104-124.
  29. Sowell ER,Thompson PM,Welcome SE,Henkenius AL,Toga AW,Peterson BS (2003): Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder.
  30. Sowell, E., Thompson, P., Welcome, S., & Henkenius, A. (2003). Cortical abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. The Lancet, 362(9397), 1699-1707.
  31. Stroop, J. (1935). The Basis of Ligon's Theory. The American Journal Of Psychology, 47(3), 499. doi: 10.2307/1416349
  32. Vaidya C.J. (2011) Neurodevelopmental Abnormalities in ADHD. In: Stanford C., Tannock R. (eds) Behavioral Neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Its Treatment. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, vol 9. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg
  33. What Is ADHD?. (2019). Retrieved from https://www.psychiatry.org/patients-families/adhd/what-is-adhd
  34. Winsler, A., Diaz, R. M., Atencio, D. J., McCarty, E. M., & Chabay, L. (1999). Mother-child interaction, private speech, and task performance in preschool children with behavior problems, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 891-904.
Image of Alex Wood
This essay was graded by
Alex Wood
Essay’s grade:
Good
What’s grading
minus plus
Expert Review
The essay gives a good overview of inhibitory control and ADHD, but its structure and use of synonyms could be strengthened. The essay has a clear focus, but the sentence structure and grammar could be improved to make the text more concise and clear. Additionally, the voice of the essay is not consistent throughout.
minus plus
What can be improved
In terms of organization, the essay is generally well-structured, with clear introduction and conclusion sections and logically organized main points. However, some paragraphs could benefit from more focused topic sentences to better orient the reader. The essay maintains a strong focus on the topic of inhibitory control deficits in ADHD and supports its arguments with relevant research and theories. However, there are some places where the essay could benefit from greater clarity and precision in language and argumentation. The sentence structure is generally strong, although there are some run-on sentences and a few instances of awkward phrasing. In addition, the essay could benefit from more varied sentence structures to increase readability and engagement. The grammar is generally sound, but there are some errors in subject-verb agreement and a few typos that could be corrected with careful proofreading. The voice of the essay is professional and academic, but could benefit from a more engaging and authoritative tone. Additionally, the essay could benefit from stronger transitions between paragraphs to better guide the reader through the argument. Overall, the essay demonstrates a solid understanding of the topic but could be improved with more attention to language and argumentation.

Cite this Essay

Reduction Of Inhibitory Control In People With ADHD. (2021, March 18). GradesFixer. Retrieved December 8, 2024, from https://gradesfixer.com/free-essay-examples/reduction-of-inhibitory-control-in-people-with-adhd/
“Reduction Of Inhibitory Control In People With ADHD.” GradesFixer, 18 Mar. 2021, gradesfixer.com/free-essay-examples/reduction-of-inhibitory-control-in-people-with-adhd/
Reduction Of Inhibitory Control In People With ADHD. [online]. Available at: <https://gradesfixer.com/free-essay-examples/reduction-of-inhibitory-control-in-people-with-adhd/> [Accessed 8 Dec. 2024].
Reduction Of Inhibitory Control In People With ADHD [Internet]. GradesFixer. 2021 Mar 18 [cited 2024 Dec 8]. Available from: https://gradesfixer.com/free-essay-examples/reduction-of-inhibitory-control-in-people-with-adhd/
copy
Keep in mind: This sample was shared by another student.
  • 450+ experts on 30 subjects ready to help
  • Custom essay delivered in as few as 3 hours
Write my essay

Still can’t find what you need?

Browse our vast selection of original essay samples, each expertly formatted and styled

close

Where do you want us to send this sample?

    By clicking “Continue”, you agree to our terms of service and privacy policy.

    close

    Be careful. This essay is not unique

    This essay was donated by a student and is likely to have been used and submitted before

    Download this Sample

    Free samples may contain mistakes and not unique parts

    close

    Sorry, we could not paraphrase this essay. Our professional writers can rewrite it and get you a unique paper.

    close

    Thanks!

    Please check your inbox.

    We can write you a custom essay that will follow your exact instructions and meet the deadlines. Let's fix your grades together!

    clock-banner-side

    Get Your
    Personalized Essay in 3 Hours or Less!

    exit-popup-close
    We can help you get a better grade and deliver your task on time!
    • Instructions Followed To The Letter
    • Deadlines Met At Every Stage
    • Unique And Plagiarism Free
    Order your paper now